Syracuse University, USA
2017年第5卷第1期(总第6期)
65-94
2017-10-17
This study examines the acquisition of variation from the vernacular Syrian Arabic input of 22 parents in the output of their 21 children in the village of Oyoun Al-Wadi in Syria, using the four rural vowel variables (o), (o:), (e), and (e:). Each variable has two realizations: rural [o, o:, e, e:] respectively and urban [a, a:, a, a:] respectively. Fathers use the rural vowels more than mothers, but the difference is statistically insignificant. Like fathers, boys use more rural vowels than girls. However, the difference between boys and girls is statistically significant. No correlation emerged between the children’s and parents’ use of the variants, indicating that children are not acquiring their parents’ exact frequencies, which suggests developmental effect rather than statistical learning of parental input effect. The boys’ higher use of the rural forms after age eight is attributed to a social, psychological polarization process between boys and girls to create a highly differentiated gendered linguistic behavior in line with another highly differentiated gendered linguistic behavior related to a stereotypical consonant variable, (q), which is observed in both parents and children.
child dialect acquisition, variation, gender polarization, statistical learning, socio-psychological factors, Syrian Arabic, rural vs. urban vowels
doi: 10.26478/ja2017.5.6.4
al-Nassir, A. A. 1993. Sibawayh the phonologist: A critical study of the phonetic and phonological theory of Sibawayh as presented in his treatise Al-Kitab [M]. London: Kagan Paul International.
Andersen, E., et al. 1999. Cross-linguistic evidence for the early acquisition of discourse markers as register variables [J]. Journal of Pragmatics, 31:1339–1351.
Barbu, S., Martin, N. & J. P. Chevrot. 2014. The maintenance of regional dialects: a matter of gender? Boys, but not girls, use local varieties in relation to their friends’ nativeness and local identity [J].Frontiers in Psychology, 5:1–11.
Behnstedt, P. 1997. Sprachatlas von Syrien [M]. Wiesbaden: Harrassawitz.
Cameron, R. 2010. Growing up and apart. Gender divergences in a Chicagoland elementary school [J]. Language variation and change,22(2):279–319.
Chambers, J. K. 1992. Dialect acquisition [J]. Language, 68(4):673–705.
______. 2002. Dynamics of dialect convergence [J]. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 6:117–130.
Chevrot, J. P. & P. Foulkes. 2013. Introduction: Language acquisition and sociolinguistic variation [J]. Linguistics, 51(2): 251–254.
Cremona, C. & E. Bates. 1977. The development of attitudes toward dialect in Italian children [J]. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,6(3): 223–232.
Díaz-Campos, M. 2005. The emergence of adult-like command of sociolinguistic variables: A study of consonant weakening in Spanish-speaking children [A]. In David Eddington (ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages [C]. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, 56–65.
Docherty, G. J., P. Foulkes, J. Tillotson & D. J. L. Watt. 2006. On the scope of phonological learning: issues arising from socially-structured variation [A]. In L. Goldstein, D. H. Whalen & C. T. Best (eds.), Laboratory Phonology 8 [C], 393–421. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Fabes, R. A., C. L. Martin& L. D. Hanish. 2004. The next 50 years: Considering gender as a context for understanding young children’s peer relationships [J]. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50:260–273.
Foulkes, P., G. J. Docherty & D. Watt. 2005. Phonological variation in child-directed speech [J]. Language, 8(1): 177–206.
Ghimenton, A., J. P. Chevrot & J. Billiez. 2013.Language choice adjustments in child production during dyadic and multiparty interactions: A quantitative approach to multilingual interactions [J]. Linguistics, 51(2):413–438.
Habib, R. 2011. Meaningful variation and bidirectional change in rural child and adolescent language [J].University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 17(2): 81-90. Available at: http://repository. upenn.edu/pwpl/vol17/iss2/10.
______. 2012.’Imalaandroundingin a rural Syrian variety: Morpho-phonological andlexical conditioning [J].Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 57(1): 51-75.
______. 2014. Vowel variation and reverse acquisition in rural Syrian child andadolescent language [J].Language Variation and Change,26(1): 45-75.
______. 2016a. Bidirectional linguistic change in rural child and adolescent language inSyria [J].Dialectologia,16: 117-141. Available at:http://www.raco.cat/index.php/Dialectologia/article/view/ 306619/396614.
______. 2016b. Identity, ideology, and attitude in Syrian rural child and dolescentspeech [J].Linguistic Variation16(1): 34-67.
______. 2017. Parents and their children’s variable language: Is it acquisition or more?[J]. Journal of Child Language, 44(3): 628-649. (First view/Published online March 11, 2016). doi: 10.1017/ S0305000916000155.
Kerswill, P. 1996. Children, adolescents, and language change [J]. Language Variation and Change,8(2): 177–202.
Kerswill, P. & A. Williams. 2000. Creating a New Town koine: Children and language change in Melton Keynes [J]. Language in Society, 29:65–115.
Khattab, G. 2013. Phonetic convergence and divergence strategies in English-Arabic bilingual children [J]. Linguistics, 51(2): 439–472.
Labov, W. 1964. Stages in the acquisition of Standard English [A]. In R. Shuy, A. Davis & R. Hogan (eds.), Social Dialects and Language Learning [C]. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, 77–104.
______. 1972. Sociolinguistic patterns [M]. Oxford: Blackwell.
______. 1989. The child as linguistic historian [J]. Language Variation and Change,1: 85–97.
______. 2001. Principles of linguistic change[M]. Volume II: Social Factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
______. 2010. Principles of linguistic change[M]. Volume III: Cognitive and cultural factors. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.
______. 2012. What is to be learned? The community as the focus of social cognition [J]. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 10(2):265–293.
Martin, C. & R. Fabes. 2001. The stability and consequences of young children’s same-sex interactions [J]. Developmental Psychology, 37:431–446.
Miller, K. 2013. Acquisition of variable rules: /s/-lenition in the speech of Chilean Spanish-speaking children and their caregivers [J]. Language Variation and Change, 25:311–340.
Nardy, A., J. P. Cheverot, & S. Barbu. 2014. Sociolinguistic convergence and social interactions within a group of preschoolers: A longitudinal study [J]. Language Variation and Change, 26:273–301.
Patterson, J. L. 1992. The development of sociolinguistic phonological variation patterns for (ing) in young children [D]. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico dissertation.
Poplack, S. 1978. Dialect acquisition among Spanish-English bilinguals [J]. Language in Society, 7:89–103.
Roberts, J. 1994. Acquisition of variable rules: (-t, d) deletion and (ing) production in preschool children [D]. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania dissertation.
______. 1997a. Hitting a moving target: Acquisition of sound change in progress by Philadelphia children [J]. Language Variation and Change,9(2): 249–266.
______. 1997b. Acquisition of variable rules: a study of (-t, d) deletion in preschool children [J]. Journal of Child Language, 24(2): 351–372.
______. 2005. Acquisition of variation [A]. In Martin Ball (ed.), Clinical sociolinguistics [C]. Oxford: Blackwell, 153–164.
Roberts, J. & W.Labov. 1995. Learning to talk Philadelphian: Acquisition of short a by preschool children [J]. Language Variation and Change, 7: 101–112.
Romaine, S. 1995. Bilingualism. Oxford: Blackwell.
Shin, N. L. 2012. Variable use of Spanish subject pronouns by monolingual children in Mexico [A]. In K. Geeslin & M. Díaz-Campos (eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 14th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium [C]. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, 130–141.
Shin, N. L. & D. Erker. 2015. The Emergence of Structured Variability in Morphosyntax: Childhood Acquisition of Spanish Subject Pronouns [A]. In A. M. Carvalho, R. Orozco & N. L. Shin (eds.), Subject pronoun expression in Spanish: A cross-dialectal perspective [C]. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, 169–190.
Smith, J., M. Durham & L. Fortune. 2007. ‘Mam, my trousers is fa’in doon!’: Community, caregiver and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect [J]. Language Variation and Change,19(1): 63–99.
______. 2009. Universal and dialect-specific pathways of acquisition: Caregivers, children, and t/d deletion [J]. Language Variation and Change, 21: 69–95.
Smith, J., M. Durham & H. Richards. 2013. The social and linguistic in the acquisition of sociolinguistic norms: Caregivers, children, and variation [J].Linguistics, 51(2): 285–324.
Starks, D. & D. Bayard. 2002. Individual Variation in the Acquisition of Postvocalic /r/: Day Care and Sibling Order as Potential Variables [J]. American Speech, 77(2): 184–194.
Tagliamonte, S. & S. Molfenter. 2007. How’d you get that accent? Acquiring a second dialect of the same language [J]. Language in Society, 36:649–675.
Wolfram, W., P. Carter & B. Moriello. 2004. Emerging Hispanic English: New dialect formation in the American South [J]. Journal of Sociolinguistics,8(3):339–358.